|
. |
Why are men
reporting more sexual partners than women? Elina
Haavio-Mannila and J.P. Roos, In a recent article in New York Times, The
myth the math and sex, Gina Kolata
(2007) takes up the well known fact that men and women report widely
different
numbers of sex partners although this is mathematically impossible. This result is true all over the world. The
discrepancy has been interpreted by referring to evolutionary benefits,
norms,
attitudes, and status aspirations. According to evolutionary theory,
among
primates and humans, the ascension to status holds higher reproductive
returns
for males than it does for females. High status entails greater access
to desirable
things, in this case, sexual access to women (Jonason 2007; Henrich
&
Gil-White 2001; Buss 1989). For women the quality of partners is more
important
because they have a higher level of obligatory investment in their
sexual
encounters and thus would be wearier about a short-term mating man
(Buss &
Schmitt 1993; Trivers 1972). Women have more control over their sexual
success
than men because, according to sexual economics, women are the
commodity that
men compete over for access (Baumeister
& Vohs 2004). There are more popular explanations to this discrepancy,
too. David Gale,
a This problem is one of the most troublesome issues that
self-reporting
genreates in sex research (McConaghy, 1999). According to behavioral
explanations have focused on how men use prostitutes more often than
women,
that men start having sex earlier than women, or that men are more
sexually
assertive than women (Jonason 2007). Some authors have argued that the
difference is itself an artifact of reporting biases (Brown &
Sinclair,
1999; Wiederman, 1997). For example, men tend to use large round
numbers when
estimating their sexual success. When participants were told that
lie-detection
was possible, and assumedly this caused them be honest, the sex
difference
became negligible (Alexander & Fisher, 2003). Similarly when the
question
regarding number of sex partners is vague, men tend to report more sex
partners
because they self-define more acts as sex than women (Sanders &
Reinisch,
1999). But these explanations do not address why men would over-report
if not
otherwise instructed. Jonason (2007a, b, c) gives psychological
explanations. He suggests that men
may gauge their own status by comparing themselves to other men in
terms of how many sexual partners they have had. In so doing, men who
have had
more sexual partners appear to have higher status than a man with fewer
past
sexual partners. In his experimental studies he found that men were
more likely
than women to use their perceived amount of sexual success as a means
of
assessing their status. He also showed that men viewed sexual success
as more prestigious
than women. He suggests that men may be more likely to boost reports
about
their sex life both in real-life and in surveys as functions of 1)
their
perception that with more sex comes more prestige and 2) the desire to
enhance
their perceived status among others. Statistics
on the gender gap in the number of sexual partners Thanks
to the spread
of reliable contraceptive methods, it is relatively safe to have sexual
experiences with several partners without fear of unwanted pregnancy or
sexually
transmitted infections. According to a web survey on sexual
attitudes and behaviour (Durex 2005) people around the world
have an average number of nine sexual
partners. Men have had more sexual partners than women – 10.2 compared
to 6.9. More scientifically collected data
is available from surveys conducted in
1989- (Table 1). For four European areas: A
suggestion to solve the problem In this article we first show that the gender
difference in the reported number of sexual partners in lifetime is
restricted
to people with numerous sexual partners only. Men and women with a
small number
of partners report equivalent numbers of partners. Secondly, we examine
what is
the social and sexual background of the “multiamourous” or polygamic
people. Is
having multiple partners a socially determined phenomenon or is it a
personal,
perhaps biological characteristic? The
gender gap in reported numbers of sex
partners has constantly bothered researchers as was discussed above.
Clearly
men have a tendency to over-report and women to underreport their
sexual
partners. One possible hypothesis is that men
and women have a different
definition of what having a sex partner means (eg. as in the case of
Clinton-Lewinsky). This is not borne out by the data of the Finnish sex
survey,
in which the verbal definitions of sexual intercourse given by men and
women
were identical. Instead, we recently found another explanation. Inspired by Kolata’s article we checked in our data
whether the difference is the same with those who have few partners and
those
who have many. It is plausible that those who have had few partners
remember
the number of partners better than those who have had many. But also,
those who
have had few partners are in a sense “losers”; their potential for
producing
offspring is lower. Men have a higher pressure than women to have more
sexual
partners, according to evolutionary theory. The number of partners is
directly
related to their social position, for instance. On the other hand, men and
women who have had many partners are the “winners” and they have
perhaps less
pressure to change the reported numbers from the real ones, at least
when they
are men. On the other hand they have more difficulty to remember the
precise
number and therefore they might round the number off in the preferred
direction. In all our data sets, men tend to report even numbers (5,
10, 20, 30
etc.) much more often than women do. We
hypothesized that those who have had few partners remember the number
of partners better than those who have had many. Those with many
partners have
more difficulty to remember the precise number and would tend to round
the
number off, with each sex going in the preferred direction. This is
coming out from our data from (Table 2, Figure 1). We
found that people with fewer than twenty partners, both men and
women, reported about five partners. Those who reported having at least
twenty
partners (15 percent in One possible hypothesis was that men and women have
a different definition of what having a sex partner means (e.g. as in
the case
of Bill Clinton), but this is not borne out by the data. The verbal
definitions
of sexual intercourse given by men and women in our Finnish study in
1999 are
identical. So the question remains, who is cheating, men or women? We assume that the men have no reason to
over-report the numbers of their life-time sex partners, except for
occasional
rounding off, whereas women have a very strong interest in
underreporting. They
should not be seen as being promiscuous. We did a further check
controlling for
education, under the assumption that it is even more important for
highly
educated, i.e. more powerful and independent women to under-report. The
opposite hypothesis is also possible: women in a power position have no
need to
underreport. In In (Table 3, Figure 2) In the less than twenty partners’ group the more
educated people have higher number of partners and gender difference is
small. It
also small among the lower educated Swedes, but in In the groups of more than twenty partners,
education is related to number of partners in reverse ways. In In any case, it seems clear that a relatively small
minority of men and women are responsible for the difference in
misreporting
for sex partners and that the number of partners has an important role
here. Who are
the people with many partners? Our second research question is: What kind of
people are those who have multiple (20+) sexual partners? We first
compare
Swedish and Finnish men and women with multiple and few partners as
there is
more comparable data from these countries. Then we include also Women with many partners are in both countries
younger than those with less than twenty partners whereas men are older
(Tables
4-5). People with multiple partners have had their first intercourse at
a
younger age than the others. They had a slightly more education than
the
others, and had been married more times. In (Tables 4 and 5)
In Having had multiple partners was connected to
sexually liberated and versatile lifestyle, more so in Correlation and regression analyses of determinants
of numbers of partners in the four areas reveal that in References Alexander,
M. G., & Fisher, T. D. (2003). Truth and consequences: Using the
bogus
pipeline to examine sex differences in self-reported sexuality. The Journal of Sex Research, 40(1), 27-35.
Baumeister,
R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Sexual economics: Sex as woman
resource for
social exchange in heterosexual interactions. Personality
& Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 339-363. Brown, N.
R., & Sinclair, R. C. (1999). Estimating lifetime sexual partners:
Men and
women do it differently. The Journal of
Sex Research, 36, 292-297. Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences
in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Psychological
Review, 100, 204-232. Buss, D.
M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An
evolutionary
perspective on human mating. Psychological
Review, 100(2), 204-232. Durex Sex Survey 2005. www.durex.com/gss Henrich, J.
& Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely
conferred
deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural
transmission. Evolution & Human Behavior, 22, 165-196. Jonason, P.
K. (2007a). An evolutionary
perspective on sex differences in exercise behaviors and motivations. Journal
of Social Psychology, 147(1), 5-14. Jonason, P.
K. (2007b) A mediation
hypothesis to account for the sex difference in reported number of
sexual
partners: An intrasexual competion approach. Jonason, P.
K. (2007c) The power of
prestige: Why men report having more sex partnersthan women. Laumann,
Edward O., John H. Gagnon, Robert T. Michael, and Stuart Michaels
(1994), The Social Organization of Sexuality. Sexual
Practices in the United States.Chicago and Leridon,
Henri, Gertjan van Zessen & Michel Hubert (1998), The Europeans and
Their
Sexual Partners. In Hubert, Michel, Bajos, Natalie & Sandfort, Theo
eds. Sexual Behaviour and HIV/AIDS in Kolata, Gina (2007),
The Myth, the Math, the Sex. The New York
Times August 12, 2007. Haavio-Mannila, Elina
& Osmo Kontula (2003) Sexual Trends
in the Baltic Sanders, S. A. & Reinisch, J. M.
(1999). Would you say you
"had
sex" if . . . ? Journal of the American Medical
Association, 281,
275-277. Trivers, R.
(1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.),
Sexual selection and the descent of man:
1871-1971 (136-179). Wellings, Kaye, Julia
Field, Anne M. Johnson & Jane Wadsworth (1994), Sexual
Behaviour in Wiederman,
M. (1997) The truth must be in here somewhere: examining the gender
discrepancy
in self-reported lifetime number of sex partners. The
Journal of Sex Research, 34(4), 375-387. Back to beginning |