SIX YEARS OF design experiments using concept mapping - at the beginning and at the end of each of 23 learning projects  (the paper as it was really presented in the session)

Mauri Åhlberg and Vuokko Ahoranta, University of Helsinki, Finland

Email: mauri.ahlberg@helsinki.fi    

Homepage:  http://www.helsinki.fi/people/mauri.ahlberg

 

 

Abstract.  Results of six year long period of field testing of concept mapping are  described. There has been published very few long term series of concept mapping experiments. Most publications report very short experiments. In our research there were two three years long experimental periods. They were partial replications. During each period the pupils were from grade levels 4 – 6 (10 – 12 years old). Two intact school classes were used. There were 20 pupils in both classes. Concept mapping clearly revealed that pupils learnt during each learning project. Number of relevant concepts and number of relevant propositions were used as indicators of meaningful learning. At both times there were statistically significant differences between those indicators at the beginning and at the end of learning projects. Pupils were happy to see by themselves that they really learned during learning projects. It seemed to increase their self-esteem and happiness with teaching-studying-learning process. For teacher this was constructivism at its best. She could easily and clearly monitor and promote her pupils metalearning and metathinking.  Qualitative research results of the research project include teacher’s observations, her tentative theory of benefits and drawbacks of concept mapping after six years experimenting. In the preliminary quantitative analysis reliability was estimated, and it was statistically tested how much 1) shared teaching-studying-learning time, 2) sex and 3) earlier school achievement levels account for variation of the two selected indicators of meaningful learning.    

1        Introduction

There are very few long term field experiments with concept mapping. Most published articles concern only relatively short term experiments. We have studies from 1997 to 2003 and even after that with design experiments in which there is concept mapping at the beginning and at the end of the learning project. Sometimes even in the middle of it. For most of the teachers, concept mapping is an innovation.  The second author of this paper heard about concept mapping only a couple of days before she decided to join research project and learnt to use concept maps. She had no previous conceptions or theory of concept mapping.  According to our observations, concept mapping is often used only for a short experimenting period.  This means that we cannot know about  how things would develop in long-term use. We have not seen any tentative theories of classroom teachers after they have used concept mapping. But after six years of experimenting that kind of tentative theory was constructed by an experimenting classroom teacher. Neither we have seen any research on how much previous school achievement, teaching-studying-learning time and sex account for variation of indicators of meaningful learning.

 

There are different conceptions of what is concept mapping and meaningful learning and its indicators. We have experimented with a version of concept mapping developed by Ahlberg (1993 – 2005). He calls it an improved method of concept mapping. He clearly admits that he builds on the work done by Novak and his research groups in USA. However there are also many differences between these two versions of concept mapping. One oh the most important ones is that in the improved method of concept mapping concept maps are allowed to be built also from the center out wards.  Then both teacher and pupils interpret concept maps as pyramids seen above. It can be easily observed whether there is conceptual hierarchy or not. This is the only information we have been interested in hierarchies. We have taken as a starting point modern cognitive science view that concepts are elements of learning and thinking. Propositions are made out of concepts. That is why we calculate number of relevant concepts and relevant propositions and regard them as indicators of meaningful learning. For sure conceptual hierarchies are also important, but on the other hand according to modern science the world is a system. Then our best evolving tentative theories of the world have to be conceptual systems. They may not always be hierarchies, but we think that truthfulness of our conceptions  is more important than hierarchical presentations.

 

We have following research problems in mind:

1)         After six years of design experiments with concept mapping, what kind of theory teacher has constructed about benefits and drawbacks of concept mapping?

2)         How reliable is long term concept mapping?

3)         How much does following variables account for variation of selected indicators of meaningful learning: a) the shared teaching-studying-learning time, b) prior school achievement level and c) sex of pupils?

 

2          Methods

Research design and subjects

 

There were two three years long periods of experimenting, together six years of experimenting resulting totally 23 learning projects and over 46 pupil made concept maps. Together 1395  individual concept maps have been collected and analyzed. For more intensive study we have selected 9 (3+3+3) pupils of each threeyear period. They were partial replicates to each other in many senses. Intensively we have studied 414 concept maps.


 

School years

Grade levels

Age

Number of

learning projects per school year

1997 – 2000

4 - 6

10 - 13

5

2000 - 2003

4 - 6

10 - 13

3

 

 

The first school class had of 20 pupils. They started at the beginning of their fourth grade, when they were 10 years old. The last concept mapping experiments were done at the end of their sixth grade when they were 12-13 years old. During the school year 1997 – 1998 they had five learning projects were individual concept maps were collected. The next school year 1998 – 1999 they had also five learning projects were concept mapping was used in the similar way. During the third school year pupils had four learning projects in which concept mapping was used at the beginning and at the end of the learning projects. In the second intact school class there were also 20 pupils. During three school years 2000 – 2003, they had three learning projects per year, totally nine learning projects. Pupils constructed individual concept maps at the beginning and at the end of each learning project. Individual Vee heuristics were also constructed in 20 of the 23 learning projects, but we do not analyze that data here. Examples of collected data are presented in the figures 1 – 3.

 

School itself is an ordinary municipal comprehensive school in Eastern Finland. Teacher who collected data in her own classroom used data also for her doctoral (PhD) thesis in the Applied Sciences of Education.  

 

Examples of collected data are presented in the figures 1 – 3.

 

 

Figure 1. The first concept map of the male pupil (code: 208) in the beginning of the design experiment of Atmosphere. The sum of relevant concepts is 4. The sum of relevant propositions is three. ‘Atmosphere’ is the most central concept


 

 

Figure 2. The last concept map of the male pupil (code: 208) in the end of the design experiment of Atmosphere. The sum of relevant concepts is 11. The sum of relevant propositions is 12. The most central concept is ‘phenomena’, because it has more links (4 links) with other concepts than any other concept.

 

 

Figure 3.  An example of pupil (code 208) constructed Vee-heuristic from the design experiment of Atmosphere.

 

Atmosphere: Transforming pupil writing /essay or short answers) into a concept map

 

The teacher-made short- answer questions/tasks were as follows: 1) Name the gases the air is made of. 2) Explain how the wind is formed. 3) What do you know about air pressure? 4) How the atmosphere is important for humankind? 5) How rainbow is created? 6) How Northern lights are created? 7) What are uses of air for humans? 8) What causes air pollution and how it can be prevented? 9) How is thunderstorm created? 10) How is burning linked to earlier questions and their answers? Its content validity is high, because these short questions correspond the local curriculum, its main themes and big ideas.

 

We transformed the answers of the pupils into concept maps. As an example the answers of the pupil #208 are as follows:

1)      Name the gases the air is made of. Pupil #208 answers: “Oxygen, carbon dioxide, and hydrogen.”

2)      2) Explain how the wind is formed. Pupil #208 answers: “Wind is created when war air is replaced by cool air.”

3)       What do you know about air pressure? Pupil #208 answers: “Air pressure can be measured in the small space.”

4)       How the atmosphere is important for humankind? Pupil #208 answers: “Life on earth needs gases. Atmosphere gives people oxygen. It protects people from ultraviolet radiation.”

5)       How rainbow is created? Pupil #208 answers: “Rainbow has many colours. It is created when sun light goes through water drop.  Rainbow is created when there are at the same time both sunshine and rain.”

6)      How Northern lights are created? Pupil #208 answers: “Northern light can be seen in atmosphere. They are formed when dust particles from Sun collide in the atmosphere. Northern lights are seen in the Winter nights in the sky.”

7)      What are uses of air for humans? Pupil #208 answers: “Life on earth needs gases.”

8)       What causes air pollution and how it can be prevented? Pupil #208 answers: “Gases cause pollution, which can be diminished by reducing traffic. Gases cause pollution which can be diminished by filtering all pipes of factories, which burn oil and peat.”

9)      How is thunderstorm created? Pupil #208 answers: “Thunderstorms occur in the hot day. They are created when ice crystals touch each other in the clouds.” 

10)  How is burning linked to earlier questions and their answers? “Clouds may be linked to lightning, which is often directed to high point, which sometimes begins to fire.”

 

In the Figure 4 there is the concept map, which the inquiring teacher constructed transforming each of the pupil #208’s short-answer test’s propositions into concepts and propositions in the concept map. The appropriate hierarchy is there, because the teacher as an adult created it. But on the other hand the pupil himself created also appropriate hierarchies. There are 31 relevant concepts and 32 relevant propositions. From the pupil’s propositions we can easily see that he does not name Nitrogen as one of atmospheric gases. This is a fatal mistake, because the most of the air is Nitrogen. Also in Biology Nitrogen cycle is very important in ecosystems and production of amino acids and proteins in organisms. A clear misconception is that there could be “dust particles from Sun” and when these collide in the atmosphere Northern lights are created. These misconceptions were discussed and corrected in the classroom after the short answer tests were scored. When a pupil herself/himself constructs a concept map s/he can only take into the concept map what is in her/his metacognition. However all pupils were able to provide much more information when explicitly asked in the teacher-made short-answer test. These two methods to gather knowledge about what and how pupils learn and think are complementary. Both provide useful knowledge for both pupils themselves and for teachers.

 

 

Figure 4.  An example of a concept map which the teacher constructed transforming proposition by proposition pupil #208’s answers in the teacher-made short-answer test of ‘Atmosphere’. The number of relevant concepts is 31 and the number of relevant propositions is 30.

 

2        Results

Answer to the first research problem: After six years of design experiments with concept mapping, what kind of theory teacher has constructed about benefits and drawbacks of concept mapping?

 

At the beginning of the research project the teacher had no idea, no conception or tentative theory of concept mapping. What is presented below, she has constructed since 1997. The teacher’s tentative  theory consists of following propositions: 

1.         Concept mapping is an excellent way of finding out what pupils have in their metacognition about the theme of a learning project.

2.         For pupils it is important to know at the beginning of the learning project what do they know about the theme of the learning project.

3.         Teacher can plan and implement her teaching according to what pupils know at the beginning of the learning project.

4.         Teacher can group pupils in co-operative learning based on results of first concept maps in the way that it facilitates knowledge building in the classroom.

5.         Teacher understands deeply, broadly and more clearly what kind of learner each of her pupils is. Concept mapping is fastest and the most practical way I know to get an overview of what pupils know both individually and as a group. 

6.         Concept mapping is a good method for both teachers and pupils to avoid rote learning and to learn and think meaningfully, and it promotes collaborative knowledge building according to my observations.

7.         Concept maps are constructed individually in my classroom. Each pupil takes full responsibility of her own learning and thinking. There are times when knowledge is built collaboratively, and there are many kinds of collaborative learning, but in the end concept maps are individual products.

8.         When pupils compare their concepts maps from the beginning of the learning project and from the end of the learning project they see clearly how much they have learnt. This supports their positive self image as a learner. They feel happy and empowered. At the same time teacher becomes more empowered. 

9.         Concept mapping is a concrete way to demonstrate pupils how much they know and learn about themes that are studied at school.

10.       Concept mapping is a good tool for assessment of learning.

11.       Misconceptions are easily revealed by concept mapping.

12.       Constructing concept maps is often a flow experience for pupils. The have deep positive feelings of their learning and their concept maps. They see constructivism in practice: Increase of concepts and propositions in their thinking, understanding and learning

13.       Concept mapping promotes meaningful learning. School learning is not any more rote learning, but understanding broader wholes, its parts, and how they are connected, and contexts.

14.       Concept mapping facilitates other kinds of written composition task.

15.       Teacher can monitor and promote better learning of individual pupils.

16.       Constructing concept maps is hard and energy demanding work. There is an optimal amount in use of concept mapping. If it is used too often pupils become bored. The situation is the same with all other methods of monitoring and promoting learning

17.       Concept mapping is a method of learning to learn, which can be used any time when needed.

18.       Constructing concept maps is hard work. Usually pupils do not ask themselves to construct more concept maps. People mostly try to avoid hard intellectual work. But they do understand importance of concept mapping from time to time. 

19.       Most pupils learn quickly how to construct good concept maps. Some pupils need more help and instructions.

20.       Some good pupils try to include all they know into their concept maps. Then teacher has to teach her to select only the most relevant items. It is probably very educative experience for those otherwise advanced pupils.   

21.       If a pupil has very little or no prior knowledge of the theme, she may feel herself helpless. On the other hand when learning project proceeds and she clearly sees how much she has learnt then it does not matter that she did not know it all at the beginning

22.       Individual concept maps can be complemented by constructing also from time to time collaboratively or co-operatively constructed concept maps. It seems to promote shared understanding and learning.

 

 

 

Answer to the second research problem: How reliable is long term concept mapping?

 

During the first research period of three years (1997 – 2000) a total reliability estimate can be calculated from sums of relevant concepts and relevant propositions.  The resulting Cronbach’s aplha is 0.88 if calculated from raw scores, but from standardised scores the same estimate is 0.96. There is a very big variation in the raw scores. The lowest sum of relevant concepts is only 62 and the highest one 210. This is why standardised score is probably the best estimate.

 

During the second research period of three years (2000 – 2003)  a total reliability estimate can be calculated from sums of relevant concepts and relevant propositions.  The resulting Cronbach’s aplha is 0.93 if calculated from raw scores, but from standardised scores the same estimate is 0.99. There is a very big variation in the raw scores. The lowest sum of relevant concepts is only 49 and the highest one 395. Because of the huge variation of raw scores, standardised scores are probably the best estimates.

 

The reliability estimates are very high. If calculated separately for relevant concepts on the other hand and relevant propositions on the other hand the reliability estimates are a little bit lower (Tables 1. and 2.)

 

 

Learning projects during

1997 – 2000

 

Reliability estimates at the beginning of the learning projects

Reliability estimates at the end of the learning projects

Reliability of sums of relevant concepts calculated from  pupil’s concept maps.

a = 0.75 (a = 0.72)

a = 0.87  (a = 0.89)

Reliability of sums of relevant propositions calculated from  pupil’s concept maps.

a = 0.76 (a = 0.73)

a = 0.85 (a = 0.87)

 

Table 1. Reliability estimates separately for sums of relevant concepts and relevant propositions at the beginning and at the end of the learning projects during the three first years of research (1997 – 2000) The estimates are calculated first from raw scores. In parentheses are the estimates calculated from standardised scores.


 

Learning projects during

2000 – 2003

 

Reliability estimates at the beginning of the learning projects

Reliability estimates at the end of the learning projects

Reliability of sums of relevant concepts calculated from  pupil’s concept maps.

a =  0.87 (a = 0.88)

a =  0.90 (a = 0.93)

Reliability of sums of relevant propositions calculated from  pupil’s concept maps.

a =  0.88 (a = 0.89)

a =  0.91 (a = 0.94)

 

Table 2. Reliability estimates separately for sums of relevant concepts and relevant propositions at the beginning and at the end of the learning projects during the second period of the research (2000 – 2003) The estimates are calculated first from raw scores. In parentheses are the estimates calculated from standardised scores.

 

 

Answer to the third research problem: How much does following variables account for variation of selected indicators of meaningful learning: a) the shared teaching-studying-learning time, b) prior school achievement level and c) sex of pupils?

 

How much teaching-studying learning time accounts for variation of meaningful learning was calculated as follows.

 

During years 1997 – 2000 there was a statistically significant increase in sums of relevant concepts from the beginning of learning   projects (M = 85.44, SD = 17.10) to the end of learning projects (M = 145.44, SD = 44.93); paired samples t-test was used: t (8) = -5.732, p = 0.000. The eta squared statistic (.80) indicated a very large effect size. It means that 80 % of difference of sums of relevant concepts are explained statistically by the shared teaching-studying –learning time: from the first concept map to the last concept map of each learning project.

 

During the years 2000 – 2003 there was also a statistically significant increase in sums of relevant propositions from the beginning of learning projects (M = 83.33, SD = 17.20)  to the end of learning projects (M = 148.67, SD = 53.10), t (8) = -4.555, p = 0.002. The eta squared statistic (.72) indicated a very large effect size. It means that 72 % of variation of sums of relevant propositions are explained statistically by the shared teaching-studying –learning time: from the first concept map to the last concept map of each learning project.

 

b) How much prior school achievement accounts for variation of meaningful learning was calculated as follows. According to prior school achievement pupils were selected and categorized into three groups, three pupils in each group: the three pupils of low achievement (Group 1), the three pupils of average achievement  (Group 2) and the three pupils of high achievement (Group 3). There were nine intensively studied pupils. The one-way analysis of variance was conducted to explore the impact of prior school achievement on variation of indicators of meaningful learning (sums of relevant concepts and sums of relevant propositions).  There were no statistically significant differences found during the first research period of 1997 – 2000. It means that prior school achievement do not statistically account for variation of meaningful learning measured by concept mapping.

 

But during the second research period (2000 – 2003) prior school achievement accounted for variation of indicators of meaningful learning.  There were statistically significant differences in all four analyses: Sums of relevant concepts at the beginning of the learning projects (F (2, 8) = 9.701, p = 0,013) and at the end of learning projects (F (2, 8) = 6.123, p = 0,036); and sums of relevant propositions at the beginning of the learning projects (F (2, 8) = 9.412, p = 0,014)  and at the end of learning projects (F (2, 8) = 8.283, p = 0,019). The effect size estimates (eta squared) varied from 0.67 – 0.76. These figures indicated very large effect sizes.

 

There is no good explanation why in the first three year period prior school achievement did nor account for meaningful learning, but during the second three years period it accounted for meaningful earning. This shows only that these kinds of analyses ought to be done separately in each group. The main point is that all pupils learn as indicated by concept mapping regardless of prior learning.

 

c) The one-way analysis of variance was conducted to explore the impact of sex of pupils on variation of indicators of meaningful learning (sums of relevant concepts and sums of relevant propositions).  There were no statistically significant differences found during either the first research period of 1997 – 2000 or the second research period 2000 - 2003. It means that sex of pupils do not statistically account for variation of meaningful learning measured by concept mapping. This means that concept mapping suits as well to both sexes. Both girls and boys learn meaningfully by concept mapping.

3        Discussion

There are very few long term research projects of any innovation. Concept mapping is not an exception. We have described some of the main results of the six years long research and development project where individual concept mapping was used both at the beginning and at the end of each 23 learning projects. Also individual Vee heuristics were used, but hat data is not presented in this paper. We found that teacher developed a tentative theory, what does concept mapping mean in everyday classroom use at grade levels 4 – 6 (pupils ages 10 – 12 years). We found also that concept mapping is a sensitive tool to show also statistically significantly that there occurs plenty of meaningful learning between the beginning and the end of learning projects. That shared teaching-studying-learning time accounted very much of the variation of the selected indicators of meaningful learning, from 72 – 80 per cent. Prior school achievement did not account for variation of meaningful learning in the first three research years (1997 – 2000). But with the second group of pupils (2000 – 2003) it accounted considerably. However there is no general regularity here. Sex of pupils did not account for variation of indicators of meaningful learning. This is good news, because it shows that both females and males can use concept maps to promote meaningful learning as indicated by concept maps at the beginning and at the end of each 23 learning projects.

 

This paper is based on the version of concept mapping which has been developed by Ahlberg (1993 – 2005) and the empirical data presented in Ahoranta’s (2004) doctoral dissertation.

 

4        References

Ahlberg, M. (1993). Concept maps, Vee diagrams and Rhetorical Argumentation (RA) Analysis: Three educational theory-based tools to facilitate meaningful learning. Paper presented at The Third International Seminar on Misconceptions in Science and Mathematics. August 1- 5, 1993. Cornell University. Published electronically in the Proceedings of the Seminar, http://www.mlrg.org/proc3abstracts.html

Ahlberg, M. (1998). Education for sustainability, good environment and good life. In Åhlberg, M. & Leal Filho, W. (Eds.) Environmental Education for Sustainability: Good Environment, Good Life. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, 25 – 43.

Ahlberg, M. (2001). Concept mapping as a research method , www.metodix.com/showres.dll/en/metodit/methods/metodiartikkelit/kasitekartta_tutkimusmenetelmana/

Ahlberg, M. (2002). Translator’s postscript: Twenty years research on theory of integrating education, improved concept maps and Vee heuristics in Finland [in Finnish]. In Novak, J. Tiedon oppiminen luominen ja käyttö [Finnish translation of Learning, creating and using knowledge]. Jyväskylä: PS-kustannus, 300 – 315.

Ahlberg, M., Aanismaa, P. & Dillon, P. 2005. Education for sustainable living: Integrating theory, practice, design and development. Accepted to be published in  Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 39(2).

 Ahlberg, M. & Ahoranta, V. (2002). Two improved educational theory based tools to monitor and promote quality of geographical education and learning. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education 11(2), 119 – 137..

Ahlberg, M., Turja, L. & Robinson, J. (2003). Educational research and development to promote sustainable development in the city of Helsinki: helping the accessible Helsinki Programme 2001 – 2011 to achieve its goals. International Journal of Environment and Sustainable Development 2(2), 197 – 209.

Ahoranta, V. (2004). Oppimisen laatu peruskoulun vuosiluokilla 4-6 yleisdidaktiikan näkökulmasta käsitekarttojen ja Vee-heuristiikkojen avulla tutkittuna [Quality of learning monitored and promoted by concept mapping and Vee mapping in school grades 4 – 6]. Doctoral dissertation. University of Joensuu. Publications in Education N:o 99.